Motor Skill Adaptation Depends On the Level of Learning
Commenced in January 2007
Frequency: Monthly
Edition: International
Paper Count: 32799
Motor Skill Adaptation Depends On the Level of Learning

Authors: Herbert Ugrinowitsch, Suziane Peixoto dos Santos-Naves, Michele Viviene Carbinatto, Rodolfo NovellinoBenda, Go Tani

Abstract:

An experiment was conducted to examine the effect of the level of performance stabilization on the human adaptability to perceptual-motor perturbation in a complex coincident timing task. Three levels of performance stabilization were established operationally: pre-stabilization, stabilization, and super-stabilization groups. Each group practiced the task until reached its level of stabilization in a constant sequence of movements and under a constant time constraint before exposure to perturbation. The results clearly showed that performance stabilization is a pre-condition for adaptation. Moreover, variability before reaching stabilization is harmful to adaptation and persistent variability after stabilization is beneficial. Moreover, the behavior of variability is specific to each measure.

Keywords: Adaptation, motor skill, perturbation, stabilization.

Digital Object Identifier (DOI): doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1335022

Procedia APA BibTeX Chicago EndNote Harvard JSON MLA RIS XML ISO 690 PDF Downloads 1742

References:


[1] Izawa, J.; Rane, T.; Dochin, O.; Shadmehr, R. Motor Adaptation as a process of reoptimization. Journal of Neuroscience, v. 132, p. 28-41, 2008.
[2] C Button, Davids K, Bennet SJ, Taylor MA. Mechanical perturbation of the wrist during one-handed catching. Acta Psychologica 105 (2000) 9- 30.
[3] S.R. Caljouw, J. van der Kamp, G.J.P. Savelsberg. The impact of taskconstrains on the planning and control of interceptive hitting movements. Neuroscience Letters 392 (2006) 84-89.
[4] R.M.C. Spencer, H.N. Zelaznick Weber (slope) analyses of timing variability in taping and drawing tasks. Journal of Motor Behavior 35 (2003) 371-381.
[5] J.R. Tresilian, A.Plooy. Effects of acoustic startle stimuli on interceptive action. Neuroscience 142 (2006) 579-594.
[6] S.M. Pincus. Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity. Procedings of the National Academy of Science 88 (1991) 2297-301.
[7] N. Bernstein. The Co-ordination and Regulation of Human Movements. Oxford, Pergamon, 1967, 196 pp.
[8] E. Brenner, J.B.J. Smeets. Perceptual requirements for fast manual responses. Experimental Brain Research 153 (2003) 246-252.
[9] J.R. Tresilian, J. Oliver, T.J.Carroll. Temporal precision of interceptive action: differential effects of target size and speed. Experimental Brain Research 148 (2003) 425-438.
[10] E.J. Manoel, K.J. Connolly. Variability and the development of skilled actions. International Journal Psychological 19 (1995) 129-147.
[11] M.L. Latash, J.F. Scholz, F. Danion, G. Schöner. Structure motor variability in marginal redundant multifinger force production tasks. Experimental Brain Research 141 (2001) 153-165.
[12] G.F. Reed, F. Lynn, B.D. Meade Use of coefficient of variation in assessing variability of quantitative assays. Clinical and Diagnostic Laboratory Immunology 9 (2002) 1235-1239.
[13] A.B. Slifkin, K.M. Newell. Noise, information transmission and force variability. Journal Experimental Psychological Human Percept Performance 25 (1999) 837-851.
[14] Haken, H. Synergetics: An introduction: Nonequilibrium phase transitions and self-organization in physics, chemistry and biology (1983).
[15] Kelso, J. A. S. Phase Transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. American Journal of physiology: Regulatory, integrative, and comparative, 15, R 1000 - R 10004 (1984).
[16] J.P. Scholz, G. Schoner. The uncontrolled manifold concept: Identifying control variables for a functional taks. Experimental Brain Research 126 (1999) 289-306.
[17] A.D. Prager, J.L.Contreras-Vidal. Adaptation to display rotation and display gain distortion during drawing. Human Moviment Science 22 (2003) 173-87.
[18] S.J.A. Kelso, D.A. Engostrom. The complementary Nature. London, MIT Pres, 2006, 317pp.
[19] K.M. Newell. Change in Movement and Skill: Learning, Retention and Transfer. In: M.L.T. Latash, M.T. Turvey. with on Dexterity and its Development by N.A. Bernstein (Eds.), Dexterity and its Development. New Jersey, Mahwah. 1996, pp.393-429.
[20] E.J. Manoel, L. Basso, U.C. Corrêa, G. Tani Modulatity and hierarquical organization of action programs in human acquisition of graphic skills. Neuroscience Letters 335 (2002) 83-86.
[21] D. Timmann, S. Richter, S. Betsmann, K.T. Kalveram, J. Konczak. Predictive control of muscle responses to arm perturbations in celebellar patients. Journal Neurologycal NeurosurgPsychiatry 69 (2000) 345- 352.
[22] P.G. Zanone, J.A.S. Kelso. Learning and transfer as dynamical paradigms for behavioral change. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J, editors. Tutorials in Motor Behavior II. Amsterdam, Elsevier Science, 1992, pp.563-582.
[23] L. Li, J.M. Haddad, J. Hamil. Stability and variability may respond differently to changes in walking speed. Human Moviment Science 24 (2005) 257-267.
[24] D.O. Hebb. The organization of behavior: a neuropsychological theory. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1949, 335pp.
[25] J.C.E. Van Der Burg, J.H. Van Dieën. The effect of timing of a perturbation on the execution of a lifting movement. Human Moviment Science 20 (2001) 243-255.
[26] L.R.T. Willians, J.M Jasiewicz, R.W. Simmons Coincidence timing of finger, arm, and whole body movements. Perceptual Motor Skills 92 (2000) 535-547.
[27] S.C. Lai, G. Mayer-Kress, K.M. Newell. Information entropy and the variability of space-time movement error. Journal Motor Behavior 38 (2006) 451-466.
[28] T. Ceux, J. Wagemans, P. Rosas, G. Montage, M. Buekers. Perceptualmotor adaptations in a synchronization task: the joint effects of frequency and motion coherence manipulations. Behaviour Brain Research 168 (2006) 226-235.