Analysis of Brain Activities due to Differences in Running Shoe Properties
Commenced in January 2007
Frequency: Monthly
Edition: International
Paper Count: 32769
Analysis of Brain Activities due to Differences in Running Shoe Properties

Authors: K. Okubo, Y. Kurihara, T. Kaburagi, K. Watanabe

Abstract:

Many of the ever-growing elderly population require exercise, such as running, for health management. One important element of a runner’s training is the choice of shoes for exercise; shoes are important because they provide the interface between the feet and road. When we purchase shoes, we may instinctively choose a pair after trying on many different pairs of shoes. Selecting the shoes instinctively may work, but it does not guarantee a suitable fit for running activities. Therefore, if we could select suitable shoes for each runner from the viewpoint of brain activities, it would be helpful for validating shoe selection. In this paper, we describe how brain activities show different characteristics during particular task, corresponding to different properties of shoes. Using five subjects, we performed a verification experiment, applying weight, softness, and flexibility as shoe properties. In order to affect the shoe property’s differences to the brain, subjects run for 10 min. Before and after running, subjects conducted a paced auditory serial addition task (PASAT) as the particular task; and the subjects’ brain activities during the PASAT are evaluated based on oxyhemoglobin and deoxyhemoglobin relative concentration changes, measured by near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS). When the brain works actively, oxihemoglobin and deoxyhemoglobin concentration drastically changes; therefore, we calculate the maximum values of concentration changes. In order to normalize relative concentration changes after running, the maximum value are divided by before running maximum value as evaluation parameters. The classification of the groups of shoes is expressed on a self-organizing map (SOM). As a result, deoxyhemoglobin can make clusters for two of the three types of shoes.

Keywords: Brain activities, NIRS, PASAT, running shoes.

Digital Object Identifier (DOI): doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1337841

Procedia APA BibTeX Chicago EndNote Harvard JSON MLA RIS XML ISO 690 PDF Downloads 2250

References:


[1] Population Ageing and Development 2012, United Nations 2012. (Online). Available: http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/ publications/pdf/ageing/2012PopAgeingandDev_WallChart.pdf
[2] J. Rossier, E. French, D. Edward, C. Rivier, N. Ling, R. Guillemin, and F.E. Bloom, “Foot-shock induced stress increases -endorphin levels in blood but not brain,” Nature, vol. 270, no. 5638, 1977, pp.618–620.
[3] E. J. Golob, R. Irimajiri, A. Starr, "Auditory cortical activity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: relationship to subtype and conversion to dementia," Brain, vol.130, pp.740-752, Feb. 2007.
[4] A. I. Levey and J.Green, "Event-related potential changes in groups at increased risk for Alzheimer disease," Arch. Neurol., vol. 56, no.11, pp. 1398-1403, Nov. 1999.
[5] N. Mikuni, T. Nagamine, A. Ikeda, K. Terada, W. Taki, J. Kimura, H. Kikuchi and H. Shibsaki, "Simultaneous Recording of Epileptiform Discharges by MEG and Subdural Electrodes in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy," NeuroImage, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 298-306, May 1997.
[6] G. Winterer, F. W. Carver, F. Musso, V. Mattay, D. R. Weinberger and R. Coppola, "Complex relationship between BOLD signal and synchronization/desynchronization of human brain MEG oscillations," Human Brain Mapping, vol. 28, no. 9, pp. 805-816, Sep. 2007.
[7] D. T. Deply, M. Cope, P. van der Zee, S. R. Arridge, S. Wray, J. S. Watt, "Estimation of optical pathlength through tissue from direct time of flight measurement," Phys Med Biol, vol.33, no.12, pp. 1433-1442, 1988.
[8] E. Okada, D. T. Delpy, "Near-infared light propagation in an adult head model," II. Effect of superfical tissue thickness on the sensitivity of the near-infared spectroscopy signal. Appl Opt, vol.42, no.16, pp. 2915-2922, 2003.
[9] M. Corbetta, F. M. Meizin, S. Dobmyer, G. L. Shulman and S. E. Petersen, "Selective and divided attention during visual discrimination of shape, color and speed: Functional anatomy by positon emissiontomography," j. Neurosci., vol.11, no.8, pp. 2383-2402, 1991.
[10] H. j. Heinze, G. R. Mangun, W. Burchert, H. Hinrichs, M. Scholz, T. F. Munte, A. Gos, M. S. Gazzaniga and S.A.Hillyard, "Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans," Nature, vol. 372, pp. 543-546, 1994.
[11] G. R. Mangun, J. B. Hopfinger, C. L. Kussmaul, E. M. Fletcher and H. J. Heinze, "Covariations in ERP and PET measures of spatial selective attention in human extrastriate visual cortex" Brain Map., no.5, pp. 273-279, 1997.
[12] J. T. Coull and A. C. Nobre, "Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI," j. Neurosci., vol.18, no. 18, pp.7426-7435, 1998.
[13] A.C. Nobrea, D.R. Gitelmanb, E.C. Diasc and M. M. Mesulamb, "Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: overlapping neural systems," NeuroImages., vol. 11, no.3, pp.210-216, Mar. 2000.
[14] J. T. Coull, C. D. Frith and A.C.Nobre, "Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts," Neuropsychologia, vol. 38, pp. 808-819, 2000.
[15] M. Osaka, N. Osaka, H. Kondo, M. Morishita, H. Fukuyama, T. Aso and H. Shibasaki, "The neural basis of individual differences in working memory capacity: an fMRI study," NeuroImage, vol.18, pp.789-797, 2003.
[16] N. Osaka, M. Osaka, H. Kondo, M. Morishita, H. Fukuyama and H. Shibasaki, "The neural basis of executive function in working memory: an fMRI study based on individual differences," NeuroImage, vol. 21, pp.623-631, 2004.
[17] H. Kondo, M. Morishita, N. Osaka, M. Osaka, H. Fukuyama and H. Shibasaki, "Functional roles of the cingulo-frontal network in performance on working memory," NeuroImage, vol. 21, pp. 2-14, 2004.