Authoritarian Parenting Received from Mothers Reveals Individual Differences in Preschooler's False-belief, but not in Advanced Theory of Mind
Authors: Alejandra Rodríguez Villalobos, Michael Padilla-Mora, Jaime Fornaguera Trías
Abstract:
Remarkable changes, like the progress in the ability to understand others' minds, can be identified in several socio-cognitive dimensions between age four and seven. Recently, the parenting attitudes have been considerate as one of the potential extrinsic modifiers of these important developmental aspects. The aim of present study is to explore the relationship among authoritarian parenting attitudes and individual differences in Theory of Mind performance. The study included ninety-two Costarrican preschoolers. Six False-belief tasks, an Advanced Theory of Mind test and the Parenting Attitudes Inventory were used. The results demonstrate that participants with high and low Authoritarian Parenting Received differ in their performance on First and Second Order False-belief tasks, but not in Advanced Theory of Mind tasks. Theoretical considerations about possible explanations regarding these results are discussed and methodological limitations are considered to shed light over future directions.
Keywords: Authoritarian parenting, cognitive development, false- belief, individual differences, theory of mind, parenting.
Digital Object Identifier (DOI): doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1332224
Procedia APA BibTeX Chicago EndNote Harvard JSON MLA RIS XML ISO 690 PDF Downloads 1923References:
[1] Flavell, J.H. (1999) Cognitive Development: Children's knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21-45.
[2] Huges, C. & Leekam, S. (2004) What are links between Theory of Mind and Social Relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development.Social Development, 13 (4), 590- 619.
[3] Bangirana, P.; John, C.; Idro, R.; Opoka, R.; Byarugaba, J.; Jurek, A.; Boivin, M. (2009) Socioeconomic Predictors of Cognition in Ugandan Children: Implications for Community Interventions. PlosOne. 4, (11), 1-6.
[4] LeMonda, C.; Shannon, J.; Cabrera, N.; Lamb, M. (2004) Fathers and mothers play with their 2 and 3 year olds: Contributions to Languaje and cognitive development. Child Development, 75 (6), 1806-1820.
[5] Rosales, F.; Reznick & Zeisel, S. (2009) Understanding the role of nutrition in the brain and behavioral development of toddlers and preschool children: identifying and addressing methodological barriers. Nutritional Neuroscience, 12 (5), 190-202.
[6] Adi-Japha, E. & Klein, P. (2009) Relations Between Parenting Quality and Cognitive Performance of Children Experiencing Varying Amounts of Childcare. Child Development, 80 (3), 893-906.
[7] Guajardo, N.; Snyder, G.; Petersen, R. (2009) Relationships among parenting practices, parental stress, child behavior, and children-s social cognitive development. Infant and Child Development, 18, 37-60.
[8] Harrison, M. (in press) Executive Function, Parenting style and Theory of Mind.
[9] Vinden, P. (2001) Parenting attitudes and children-s understanding of mind. A comparison of Korean American and Anglo American families. Cognitive Development, 16, 793-809.
[10] Rapacholi, R.; Slaughter, V.; Pritchard, M.; Gibbs, V. (2003) Theory of Mind, Machiavellianism and Social Functioning in Childhood. En Rapacholi, B.; Salughter, V. (Eds) Individual differences in Theory of Mind. New York: Psychology Press.
[11] Happé, F. (1994) An advanced test of Theory of Mind: Understanding of story characters- thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24 (2), 129-154.
[12] Padilla-Mora, M. (2008) Funcionamiento coarticulado entre procesos cognitivos: algunas din├ímicas de la relaci├│n entre el control inhibitorio, la memoria de trabajo, algunas habilidades ling├╝├¡sticas y las tareas de falsa-creencia. Tesis de Maestr├¡a en Ciencias Cognoscitivas, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José.
[13] Padilla-Mora, M; Cerdas, A.; Rodríguez, O. & Fornaguera, J. (2009 ) Teoría de la Mente en niños preescolares: Diferencias entre sexos y capacidad de Memoria de Trabajo. Actualidades Investigativas en Educación, 9 (2), 1-21.
[14] Astington, J.; Pelletier, J.& Homer, B. (2002) Theory of mind and epistemological development: the relation between children's second order false-belief understanding and their ability to reason about evidence. New Ideas in Psychology, 20, 131-144.
[15] Schneider, W., Lockl, K. & Fernandez, O. (2005). Interrelationships among theory of mind, executive control, language development, and working memory in young children: A longitudinal analysis. En W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children-s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functions, working memory, verbal ability and theory of mind (pp. 259- 284). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[16] Nguyen, L. & Frye, D. (1999). Children-s theory of mind: Understanding of desire, belief and emotion with social referents. Social Development, 8(1), 70-92.
[17] Sullivan, K., Zaitchik, D. & Tager-Flusberg, H.. (1994) Preschoolers can attribute second-order belief. Developmental Psychology, 30, 395-402.
[18] Sodian, Beate & H├╝lsken, Christian. (2005). The development relation of theory of mind and executive functions: a study of advanced theory of mind abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. En Young children-s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functions, working memory, verbal ability and theory of mind (pp. 175-188). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[19] Padilla-Mora, M.; Rodríguez, O. & Fornaguera, J. (2009) Interacciones entre el entendimiento de la falsa-creencia y el desarrollo de la habilidad verbal: diferencias entre los sexos en edad preescolar. Interdisciplinaria, 26 (2), 317-344.
[20] O-Hare, A.; Bremmer, L.; Nash, M.; Happé, F.; Pettigrew, L. (2009) A clinical assessment tool for Advanced Theory of Mind performance in 5 to 12 years olds. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39 (9), 916-928.
[21] White, S.; Hill, E.; Happé, F; Frith, U. (2009) Revisiting the Strange Stories: Revealing Mentalizing Impairments in Autism. Child Development, 80 (4), 1097-1117.
[22] Stevens, J.P. (1992). Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences (2nd edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[23] Anderson, J.; Pellowsky, M.; Conture, E.; Kelly, E. (2003) Temperamental characteristics of young children who stutter. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 46 (5), 1221-1233.
[24] Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.
[25] Cyr, C. & Dubois-Comtois, K. (2008) Les conversations mère-enfant et l-attachment des enfants ├á la periode préscolaire. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 40 (3), 140-152.
[26] Pears, K. & Moses, L. (2003) Demographics, parenting and Theory of Mind in preschool children. Social Development, 12 (1), 1-20.
[27] Vega, I. & Cordero, A. (2001) Realidad familiar en Costa Rica: aportes y desafíos desde las ciencias sociales. San José: FLACSO.
[28] Villanueva, L., Clemente, R. & García, F. (2000) Theory of mind and peer rejection at school. Social Development, 9 (3), 271-283.
[29] Levitt, M. (2005) Social relations in childhood and adolescence: The Convoy model perspective. Human Development, 48, 28-47.
[30] Rogers, K.; Dziobek, I.; Hassenstab, J.; Wolf, O.; Convit, A. (2007) Who cares? Revisiting Empathy in Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 709-715.
[31] Kazdin, A. (1992) Cognitive problem-solving skills training and parent management training in the treatment of antisocial behavior in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60 (5), 733-747.
[32] Fisher, P., Gunnar, M., Chamberlain, P. y Reid, J. (2000) Preventive intervention for maltreated preschool children: Impact on children’s behavior, neuroendocrine activity, and foster parent functioning. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39 (11), 1356-1364.